Journal of Student Research (2013)

Volume 2, Issue 1: pp.

52-57

Research Article

a. Winthrop University, Rock Hill, SC, 29733

www.jofsr.

com

52

Relations Among Parentification, Parenting Beliefs, and

Parenting Behaviors

Brittney T.

Black

a

& Merry J.

Sleigh

a

Instrumental parentification occurs when children take on adult chores, and emotional parentification occurs when children take

the responsibility for providing an adult-level of emotional support.

This study examined how the level of parentification and

perceived unfairness in the childhood home affected adults’ parenting beliefs and future parenting behaviors.

One hundred and

seven young adults responded to the Parentification Questionnaire which assessed levels of instrumental parentification,

emotional parentification, and perceived fairness.

Participants were asked to imagine themselves as parents and then respond to

questions that assessed their parenting beliefs, planned parenting behaviors, and demographics.

Results revealed that emotional

parentification and perceived unfairness were related to adults’ decreased self-esteem and feelings of attractiveness.

In addition,

parentification and perceived unfairness in the childhood home were linked to negative feelings toward participants’ own parents;

however, parentification did not clearly predict factors associated with parenting.

Keywords: Parentification; Parenting; Behaviors; Beliefs

Introduction

Parentification occurs when children are required to

function as an adult when it is developmentally inappropriate

for them to do so (Wells & Jones, 2000).

The practice of

parentification has been occurring for centuries but only

within the last few decades have researchers associated a

name with it, and research on this topic is limited.

There are

two types of parentification: emotional and instrumental.

Emotional parentification occurs when children provide

emotional support to other family members, such as being

treated as a parent’s friend and confidant (Jurkovic, Thirkfield

& Morrell, 2001).

Instrumental parentification occurs when

children take on family tasks that are typically done by adults,

such as grocery shopping or caring for siblings (Jurkovic et al,

2001).

Parentification often co-occurs with a sense that the

home environment is unfair to them, termed ‘perceptions of

fairness’ (Jurkovic et al, 2001).

This phenomenon is seen

across cultures (e.

g., Shih, Wu, & Lin, 2010; Titzmann, 2012)

and has been linked to both negative and positive outcomes in

adulthood (Carroll & Robinson, 2000; Hooper, Marotta, &

Lanthier 2008; Jurkovic et al, 2001; Peris, Goeke-Morey,

Cummings, & Emery, 2008; Wells & Jones, 2000).

For

example, adults who experienced parentification during

childhood have an increased likelihood of engaging in

substance abuse (Hooper, DeCoster, White & Voltz, 2011),

but also are likely to exhibit resilience in the face of negative

events (Hooper, Marotta, & Lanthier 2008).

The goal of the

current study was to further our understanding of this

relatively new construct by examining whether the childhood

experience of parentification relates to adults’ beliefs about

their own parenting.

Literature Review

Parentification has been associated with several factors

that are believed to be causal.

Parentification has been

consistently linked to family unpredictability, parental

alcoholism, poor parental health, marital conflict, and low

family income (Burnett, Jones, Bliwise, & Ross, 2006;

Burton, 2007; Godsall, Jurkovic, Emshoff, Anderson, &

Stanwyck, 2004; Kelley et al, 2007; Hooper, Doehler,

Jankowski, & Tomek, 2012; Peris et al, 2008).

Not only are

there many factors that may lead to parentification, these

factors can have differential impacts.

Carroll and Robinson

(2000) found that children of workaholics and children of

alcoholics both had the potential for parentification; however,

the children of workaholics experienced significantly higher

levels of parentification than children of alcoholics.

In addition to being multi-causal, parentification is

linked to multiple adverse outcomes.

Parentification has been

linked to a variety of pathologies such as eating disorders,

mood disorders, substance abuse disorders, and personality

disorders (Hooper, DeCoster, White, & Voltz, 2011).

In

addition, children who experience parentification are

vulnerable to the imposter phenomenon, as described by

Castro, Jones, and Mirsalimi (2004).

The imposter

phenomenon occurs when individuals feel as though people’s

positive perceptions of them are inaccurate, resulting in a lack

of confidence in one’s uniqueness, talent, and the ability to

meet others’ demands (Castro et al, 2004).

Adults who

experienced parentification as a child are also vulnerable to

feelings of shame versus guilt (Wells & Jones, 2000).

Shame

is considered the more damaging of the two, because mistakes

leave a shame-prone person feeling badly about the self; in

contrast, guilt is a focus on the behavior and ways to correct

it.

Parentification may have more of an impact than parents

realize.

Peris, Goeke-Morey, Cummings, and Emery (2008)

examined parental reliance on emotional support from

children during marital conflicts.

The researchers found that

mothers tended to seek emotional support from children more

than fathers and especially from their daughters.

The results

also showed that parents reported more closeness and warmth

with their children, while children reported low levels of

warmth and support from parents.

These findings suggest that

children feel more negative effects from emotional

parentification during marital conflict than parents perceive.

Journal of Student Research (2013)

Volume 2, Issue 1: pp.

52-57

Research Article

ISSN: 2167-1907

www.jofsr.

com

53

Despite the many negative associations, previous

research has revealed some positive associations with

parentification.

Hooper, Marotta, and Lanthier (2008) found

that people who experienced parentification could have

posttraumatic growth, particularly increased resiliency.

Posttraumatic growth refers to the positive outcome that the

individual experiences after going through an aversive

situation.

In Hooper et. al’s (2008) case, the aversive situation

was parentification and as a result of experiencing

parentification, the individual was more resilient to negative

life events.

Similarly, instrumental parentification in the form

of caring for siblings was linked to psychosocial adjustment

(versus maladjustment) perhaps because the act of caregiving

increased self-efficacy in the individual (Fitzgerald,

Schneider, Zinzow, Jackson, & Fossel, 2008).

The potential

for positive outcomes may depend to some extent on the

child.

For example, Williams and Francis (2010) argued that

an internal locus of control may shield children from the

negative effects of parentification.

Despite the potential for

positive outcomes, researchers who examine both typically

find more negative effects of parentification than positive

effects, especially in regard to emotional parentification

(Black, Lindberg, Garansi, & Sleigh, 2011).

Similarly,

Hooper, Doehler, Jankowski and Tomek (2012) found that

adolescents with alcoholic parents were less likely to have

alcohol problems themselves if they were experiencing

parentification, but were more likely to exhibit depressive

symptoms.

In other words, parentification was a buffer for

one adverse outcome and a moderator for the other.

Because parentification is so closely tied with parenting,

it seems logical that childhood parentification would impact

adults’ parenting behaviors.

Hooper (2007) suggested that

parentification should be conceptualized as interference with

healthy attachment between the parent and child.

Emotional

parentification and perceived unfairness may be particularly

detrimental to the parent-child relationship, as they both

contain an emotional element.

In fact, Nutall, Valentino, and

Borkowski (2012) recently found that mothers who

experienced parentification were less warm with their young

children, resulting in those children exhibiting more

problematic

externalizing

behaviors.

Instrumental

parentification, in contrast, is more about practical matters,

such as getting dinner on the table and has even been linked to

increased interpersonal competence (Thirkield, 2002).

We examined how childhood parentification related to

adults’ parenting beliefs and behaviors.

We hypothesized that:

(1) participants who experienced parentification would

perceive their own parents in a poor light and report a desire

to raise their own children in a different way and (2) adults

who experienced parentification during their childhood would

have different parenting behaviors and beliefs than adults who

had not experienced parentification.

Our hypothesis was non-

directional because we were unsure of whether people would

repeat their experiences with childhood parentification or they

would choose a different parenting experience than their own.

Method

Participants

One hundred seven adults with a mean age of 20.

63 (sd

= 2.75) participated in the study.

Forty-seven percent

identified as Caucasian, 40% identified as African-American,

and 13% identified as another ethnicity.

Sixty-six percent of

the participants were women and 34% were men.

Participants

confirmed their status as non-parents.

This study was

approved by the university’s Institutional Review Board, and

all participation was voluntary.

Materials

We measured instrumental and emotional parentification,

and perceived fairness in participants’ childhood homes with

a modified Parentification Questionnaire (Hooper & Wallace,

2010).

The original Parentification Questionnaire was a 30

item, self-report measure developed by Jurkovic and Thirkield

(1998).

Hooper and Wallace (2010) focused on 21-items that

loaded on three constructs (instrumental parentification,

emotional parentification, and perceived fairness), and

demonstrated this modified scale as a reliable measure of

parentification; Cronbach’s alpha coefficients were over.

80

for all scales (instrumental parentification = .81, emotional

parentification = .

82, perceived unfairness = .

88).

Instrumental parentification included five items such as,

“I often did the family’s laundry” and “My parents expected

me to help discipline my siblings.

” Emotional parentification

included seven items such as, “At times I felt I was the only

one my mother or father could turn to” and “In my family I

often made sacrifices that went unnoticed.

” Perceived fairness

included nine items such as, “My parents often tried to get me

to take their sides in conflicts” and “It often seemed that my

feelings weren’t taken into account in my family.

Participants rated how true each statement was of them on a

five-point Likert scale with one representing “Strongly

Disagree” and five representing “Strongly Agree.

” Some

items were reverse-scored and a mean score was calculated

for each sub-scale (Hooper & Wallace, 2010).

The higher the

score on each sub-scale, the more of that specific type of

parentification the participants experienced in their childhood

homes.

Thirty-five additional questions/statements were created

by the researchers.

Parenting behaviors were assessed by

having participants respond with their level of agreement to

five statements such as “I try to protect my child from

growing up too fast” and “I probably do more for my child

than I should.

” Parenting beliefs were assessed by having

participants respond with their level of agreement to 15

statements such as “Children deserve an allowance” and

“Children should not question their parents.

” The parenting

belief and behavior statements were created after examining

existing lists of parenting activities and seeing many

consistent themes across them.

We also asked participants to

rank three future behaviors (caring for your spouse/partner,

caring for your child/children, having a successful career)

from the one most important to the one least important to

them. Last, participants responded to demographic questions.

Procedure

The survey was posted online through

surveymonkey.

Participants were recruited for the study

through social media using a snowball technique.

The authors

posted the link to the survey on their own social media sites,

and visitors to those sites were asked to share the link through

their social media sites.

Participants also were provided the

link through undergraduate classrooms and offered extra

credit for participating.

The survey was posted for a period of

Journal of Student Research (2013)

Volume 2, Issue 1: pp.

52-57

Research Article

ISSN: 2167-1907

www.jofsr.

com

54

two months, and participants recruited outside of the

classroom were offered no incentives for participation.

Participants first responded to the Parentification

Questionnaire, followed by the questions related to parenting

behaviors and beliefs.

Participants were asked to imagine

themselves in the role of a parent and then to respond in a

way that reflected the parenting beliefs and behaviors they

planned to use with their own child(ren).

Data was analyzed

using Pearson’s correlations.

Results

We computed a “Perceptions of Fairness” score (POF),

an “Emotional Parentification” score (EPS), and an

“Instrumental Parentification” score (IPS) for each participant

by following Hooper and Wallace’s (2010) instructions to

reverse score specified items and then calculate a mean for the

relevant questions on each dimension.

A higher POF score

indicated that participants perceived a greater sense of

unfairness in their childhood home.

For IPS and EPS, a higher

score indicated more experience with that particular type of

parentification.

We examined relationships among the sub-scales using

Pearson’s correlations.

Emotional parentification and

instrumental parentification were positively correlated, r(99)

= .48, p < .01.

POF was positively correlated with both

instrumental, r(99) = .33, p < .01 and emotional

parentification, r(102) = .66, p < .01.

These correlations,

revealing medium to large effect sizes, among the sub-scales

are consistent with previous research (e.

g., Hooper &

Wallace, 2010).

Parentification and Perceptions of Parents

Results can be seen in Table 1.

The higher the IPS, the

more participants agreed that they were unhappy with the way

they were raised, r(99) = .23, p = .023, revealing a small

effect.

The higher the EPS, the more participants agreed that

their parents were not there for them, r(99) = .

48, the more

unhappy participants felt about the way they were raised,

r(99) = .

45, the less participants agreed that they want to raise

their families in the way they were raised, r(99) = -.

49, and

the less participants agreed that their parents did the best job

they could in raising them, r(97) = -.

40 (p < .01 and revealing

medium effect sizes in all cases).

The higher the POF, the more participants agreed that

their parents were not there for them, r(99) = .

70, the more

unhappy participants felt about the way they were raised,

r(99) = .

72, the less participants agreed that they want to raise

their families in the way they were raised, r(99) = -.

71, and

the less participants agreed that their parents did the best job

they could in raising them, r(97) = -.

64 (p < .01 and revealing

large

effect

sizes

in

all

cases).

Table 1

Correlations between parentification sub-scales and perception of parents

Statement

POF

EPS

IPS

I want to raise my family in

the same way that I was

raised.

-.713

.000

-.487

.000

Overall, I fell very

negative/unhappy about the

way in which I was raised.

.720

.000

.446

.000

.229

.023

I believe that my parents

did the best job they could

in raising me.

-.642

.000

-.395

.000

My parents were not there

for me as much as I would

have liked them to be.

.690

.000

.479

.000

Parentification and Parenting Behaviors

We examined relations among variables using Pearson’s

correlations.

Instrumental

parentification,

emotional

parentification, and perceived unfairness scores did not

predict how participants responded to the list of parenting

behaviors.

In other words, a childhood experience with

parentification was not linked to a particular pattern of

participants’ planned parenting behaviors.

However we did

find that participants high in POF, ranked their career as more

important than caring for their spouse or children, r(94) = -

.23, p < .05, revealing a small effect size.

Parentification and Parenting Beliefs

The higher the EPS, the less participants agreed that it is

very important for children to do well in school, r(96) = -.

21,

p < .05.

The higher the POF, the less participants agreed with

the notion that children should not question parents, r(96) = -

.24. POF was positively correlated with the belief that parents

should respect their child’s privacy, r(96) = .21, and that

parents should always speak in a calm voice to their children,

r(96) = .

24 (p < .05 in all cases, revealing small effect sizes).

IPS did not predict participants’ parenting beliefs.

Because we did not find strong relations between

childhood experiences and parenting behaviors, we examined

participants’ overall responses to parenting situations.

The

results revealed that participants varied in their beliefs about

parenting.

Over 50% of participants were in agreement on

only nine of the 15 parenting statements (see Table 2).

Journal of Student Research (2013)

Volume 2, Issue 1: pp.

52-57

Research Article

ISSN: 2167-1907

www.jofsr.

com

55

Table 2

Percentage of Participants who Agreed with Parenting Belief Statements

Statement

Percentage of

Participants who

Agreed

Parents should tell their children “no” when needed.

98%

Parents should clearly explain the rules and expectations to their child.

94%

It is very important that children do well in school.

92%

Physical affection with children is important.

91%

Parents should get their child to try new things.

89%

Parents should have high expectations of their children.

75%

Parents should be sensitive to every one of their child’s moods.

66%

It is important for parents to teach their children to share their beliefs

(religious or cultural).

61%

Parents should respect their child’s privacy.

58%

Children need to compete with others to learn about the real world.

46%

Spanking a child is an appropriate form of discipline.

45%

Parents should always speak in a calm voice to their children.

45%

Children deserve an allowance.

31%

Children should not question their parents.

24%

It is the school’s responsibility to educate children, not the parent’s job.

3%

Post-hoc Analyses

The higher the POF, the lower the participants’ self-

esteem, r(94) = -.

37, p < .01, and the less attractive

participants rated themselves, r(95) = -.

21, p = .04, revealing

small effect sizes.

The higher the EPS, the lower the

participants’ self-esteem, r(94) = -.

27, p < .01, and the less

attractive participants rated themselves, r(95) = -.

23, p = .026,

revealing small effect sizes.

IPS did not predict participants’

self-esteem or perceptions of their own attractiveness.

Discussion

The purpose of the study was to examine whether

childhood experiences with parentification affected adults

parenting beliefs.

We hypothesized that participants who

experienced parentification would perceive their own parents

in a poor light and report a desire to raise their own children

in a different way.

This hypothesis was supported.

Participants high in instrumental parentification, emotional

parentification, or perceived fairness reported being

“unhappy” with the way in which they were raised.

Emotional

parentification and perceptions of fairness were also

associated with participants wanting to raise their own

families differently than they way in which they were raised.

In other words, childhood parentification was linked to adults’

unhappiness with their childhood home, as well as a general

desire to avoid replicating that situation with their own

children.

We also hypothesized that parentification would

influence the way adults planned to parent, and this

hypothesis seemed especially likely to be met given the

unhappiness that our parentified participants reported with

their own parents; however, our results only partially

supported this hypothesis.

Emotional parentification,

perceived fairness, and instrumental parentification levels did

not predict participants’ support of specific parenting

behaviors.

Similarly, experience with emotional parentification and

perceptions of fairness predicted limited parenting beliefs.

Participants with high EPS were less likely to agree that it

was important for children to do well in school.

Participants

with high POF disagreed that children should not question

their parents and agreed that parents should respect children’s

privacy and speak to children calmly.

Participants who felt

Journal of Student Research (2013)

Volume 2, Issue 1: pp.

52-57

Research Article

ISSN: 2167-1907

www.jofsr.

com

56

they were treated unfairly seem to be revealing parenting

beliefs that decrease the power of the parent over the child

and thus, decrease the ability for unfair treatment.

Participants

who scored high in POF also ranked career as a more

important priority than caring for their spouse of children.

Once again, these participants may be attempting to

compensate for the unfairness they perceived in their

childhood home, as lower family income is one predictor of

parentification (Burton, 2007).

Despite these associations, it is noteworthy that most

parenting beliefs did not relate to experience with

parentification.

For example, instrumental parentification was

not associated with parenting beliefs.

Out of the fifteen

presented parenting beliefs, emotional parentification was

associated with only one belief, and perceived fairness was

associated with three beliefs.

One interpretation is that an

individual’s experiences with childhood parentification do not

relate to their planned parenting in adulthood.

Another

plausible interpretation is that participants who experienced

parentification wanted to raise their own families differently

but did not share a common idea of what this might mean.

In

other words, participants may plan to be different than their

own parents in a variety of ways, with some planning to be

more permissive and others planning to be more hands-on.

In

support of this interpretation, we did not find clear consensus

in how participants responded to the parenting behavior

statements.

Taylor, Lauder, Moy and Corlett (2009) argued that even

though the details might be complicated, there is general

agreement among adults about what it means to be a “good

enough” parent.

In other words, most young adults share an

implicit idea of how good parents function.

Interestingly, our

participants who experienced parentification did not show this

expected agreement.

This lack of agreement may reflect the

fact that children experiencing parentification do not have

enough exposure to typical parenting situations that lead to

normative parenting ideas.

Burton (2007) demonstrated that

children who experience parentification are often perceived as

“different” from other children because of the uniqueness of

their life experiences.

Another possibility is that this finding

reflects yet another atypical outcome of parentification not yet

identified in previous research.

Previous research revealed that sometimes individual’s

parenting beliefs and behaviors do not match (Burchinal,

Skinner, & Reznick, 2010).

In this study, we were asking non-

parents to predict their future parenting behaviors.

Although

parentified participants shared the belief that they did not

want to be like their own parents, they may not have been

able to accurately translate that belief into accurate

predictions of their own parenting, resulting in the diversity of

responses we collected.

Perhaps if participants were actually

in a parenting situation, we would have uncovered unique

patterns of behavior as did Nutall, Valentino, and Borkowski

(2012).

Our results also revealed that emotional parentification

and perceived unfairness predicted participants’ decreased

self-esteem and perceived attractiveness, matching previous

research (Black et al, 2011).

In other words, experiences with

parentification can have long-lasting effects on feelings of

self-worth.

This finding raises questions about how

parentification can impact people’s self-image without having

any clear effect on their image of themselves as parents.

This

area is ripe for future research.

One limitation of the current study is that we studied

only non-parents.

Future studies may want to focus

specifically on people who are actually parents and compare

parenting beliefs and behaviors of non-parents to actual

parents.

Future researchers also could use observational

methodology to compare parenting behaviors of adults who

experienced parentification and those who did not, as well as

to assess how their parenting beliefs and behaviors match.

Another limitation is that we did not randomly sample

our participants from a broad population, relying instead on a

university population.

Because of their successful enrollment

at a university, these participants may not have experienced

extreme parentification, such as that of adult children of

alcoholics.

Adults in more extreme childhood situations may

have had less opportunity to witness typical parenting and

may exhibit more extreme reactions to their childhood home.

That greater impact may translate into unique parenting

beliefs.

Future studies may want to focus on more vulnerable

populations.

In sum, this data in combination with previous research,

demonstrates that parentification impacts individuals in ways

that are likely to influence their parenting, such as decreased

self-esteem.

However, parentification did not clearly predict

parenting beliefs or behaviors.

Adults who experienced

childhood parentification expressed unhappiness with the way

they were raised, yet did not report consensus in the ways

they planned to differ from their own parents.

This finding

adds to our growing understanding of parentification and

parenting.

References

Black, B.

, Lindberg, K.

, Garansi, A.

& Sleigh, M.

J. (2011).

Parentification implications: A study on positive

and negative effects of childhood parentification.

Presented at the Southeastern Psychological

Association conference, New Orleans, LA.

Burchinal, M.

, Skinner, D.

, & Reznick, J.

S. (2010).

European

American and African American mothers’ beliefs

about parenting and disciplining infants: A mixed-

method analysis.

Parenting: Science & Practice,

10(2), 79-96.

Burnett, G.

, Jones, R.

, Bliwise, N.

G., & Ross, L.

T. (2006).

Family unpredictability, parental alcoholism, and

the development of parentification.

American

Journal of Family Therapy, 34, 181-189.

Burton, L.

(2007).

Childhood adultification in economically

disadvantaged families: A conceptual model.

Family Relations, 56, 329-345.

Carroll, J.

J., & Robinson, B.

E. (2000).

Depression and

parentification among adults as related to parental

workaholism and alcoholism.

The Family Journal:

Counseling and Therapy for Couples and Families,

8, 360-367.

Castro, D.

M., Jones, R.

A., & Mirsalimi, H.

(2004).

Parentification and the imposter phenomenon: An

empirical investigation.

American Journal of

Family Therapy, 32, 205-216.

Fitzgerald, M.

M, Schneider, R.

A, Zinzow, H.

M., Jackson, J.

,

& Fossel, R.

V. (2008).

Child sexual abuse, early

Journal of Student Research (2013)

Volume 2, Issue 1: pp.

52-57

Research Article

ISSN: 2167-1907

www.jofsr.

com

57

risk, and childhood parentification: Pathways to

current psychosocial adjustment.

Journal of Family

Psychology, 22, 320-324.

Godsall, R.

E., Jurkovic, G.

J., Emshoff, J.

, Anderson, L.

, &

Stanwyck, D.

(2004).

Why some kids do well in bad

situations: Relation of parental alcohol misuse and

parentification to children’s self-concept.

Substance

Use and Misuse, 39, 789-809.

Hooper, L.

M. (2007).

The application of attachment theory

and family systems theory to the phenomena of

parentification.

The Family Journal, 15(3), 217-

223.

Hooper, L.

M., DeCoster, J.

, White, N.

, & Voltz, M.

L.

(2011).

Characterizing the magnitude of the relation

between self-reported childhood parentification and

adult psychopathology: A meta-analysis.

Journal of

Clinical Psychology, 67, 1-16.

Hooper, L.

M., Doehler, K.

, Jankowski, P.

T., & Tomek, S.

E.

(2012).

Patterns of self-reported alcohol use,

depressive symptoms, and body mass index in a

family sample: The buffering effects of

parentification.

The Family Journal, 20, 164-178.

Hooper, L.

M., Marotta, S.

A, & Lantheir, R.

P. (2008).

Predictors of growth and distress following

childhood

parentification:

A

retrospective

exploratory study.

Journal of Child and Family

Studies, 17(5), 693-705.

Hooper, L.

M., & Wallace, S.

A. (2010).

Evaluating the

parentification questionnaire:

Psychometric properties and psychopathology

correlates.

Contemporary Family Therapy: An

International Journal, 32(1), 52-68.

Jurkovic, G.

J., & Thirkield, A.

(1998).

Parentification

Questionnaire (Available from G.

J.

Jurkovic, Department of Psychology, Georgia State

University, University Plaza, Atlanta, GA 30303).

Jurkovic, G.

J., Thirkfield, A.

, & Morrell, R.

(2001).

Parentification of adult children of divorce:

A multidimensional analysis.

Journal of Youth and

Adolescence, 30, 245-257.

Kelley, M.

L., French, A.

, Bountress, K.

, Keefe, H.

A.,

Schroeder, V.

, Steer, K.

, Fals-Stewart, W.

, &

Gumienny, L.

(2007).

Parentification and family

responsibility in the family of origin of adult

children of alcoholics.

Addictive Behaviors, 32,

675-685.

Nuttall, A.

K., Valentino, K.

, & Borkowski, J.

G. (2012).

Maternal history of Parentification, maternal warm

responsiveness, and children’s externalizing

behavior.

Journal of Family

Psychology, 26(5).

Peris, T.

S., Goeke-Morey, M.

C., Cummings, E.

M., & Emery,

R.E. (2008).

Martial conflict and

support seeking by parents in adolescence:

Empirical support for the parentification construct.

Journal of Family Psychology, 22(4), 633-642.

Shih, F.

M., Wu, L.

C., & Lin, S.

H. (2010).

A correlational

study on parentification, self-differentiation, and

health for students in senior high and vocational

high school in Taiwan.

Bulletin of Educational

Psychology, 41(4), 823-846.

Taylor, J.

, Lauder, W.

, Moy, M.

, & Corlett, J.

(2009).

Practitioner assessments of ‘good enough’

parenting: Factorial survey.

Journal of Clinical

Nursing, 18(8), 1180-1189.

Thirkield, A.

(2002).

The role of fairness in emotional and

social outcomes of childhood filial responsibility.

Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Georgia State

University, Atlanta, GA.

Titzmann, P.

F. (2012).

Growing up too soon? Parentification

among immigrant and native adolescents in

Germany.

Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 41(7),

880-893.

Wells, M.

, & Jones, R.

(2000).

Childhood parentification and

shame-proneness: A preliminary study.

American

Journal of Family Therapy, 28(1), 19-27.

Williams, K.

, & Francis, S.

E. (2010).

Parentification and

psychological adjustment: Locus of control as a

moderating variable.

Contemporary Family

Therapy: An International Journal, 32(3), 231-237.